In this last part of this series, we are going to look forward to see what Current British English will look like in the near future. We can make predictions about the future because it already lives with us: young speakers represent the standard of the near future. That is, we will look at some changes going on today, spreading among younger speakers.
TH-fronting
English has eight fricatives: f v θ ð s z ʃ ʒ. Two of these are disappearing from the speech of many speakers: θ ð. As we have seen elsewhere, this development, referred to as TH-fronting, is a significant simplification of the consonant system of English, it evacuates a whole place of articulation, these two fricatives are the only dental consonants.
Many speakers replace θ by f across the board: thinkθiŋk > fiŋk, bathbaːθ > baːf, monthmənθ > mənf, throughθruw > fruw, theoryθiːrij > fiːrij, patheticpəθetik > pəfetik, faithfulfejθfəl > fejffəl. This event has an impact on phonotactic constraints too: in varieties of English without th-fronting the diphthong aw may only be followed by coronal consonants, including θ: eg mouthmawθ or southsawθ. In th-fronting, the coronal θ turns into a noncoronal consonant, f. As a result aw may now be followed no only by coronals, but also by the labial f, as in mouthmawf or southsawf.
The more frequently a word is used, the more likely this change is. Thus thirty three may be heard as θə:tij frij, ie the θ in thirty, the less frequent word, unchanged, but that in three, the more frequent one, replaced by f.
The other fricative altered by TH-fronting, ð, may have several outcomes. The most common pattern is for it to turn into v after a vowel or a glide: motherməðə > məvə, bathebejð > bejv, withoutwiðáwt > wiváwt; and into d elsewhere: thenðen > den, the catðə kat > də kat, althoughoːlðəw > oːldəw.
Palatalization has been going on for centuries in English. It affects alveolar obstruents (t d s z) turning them into their palatal counterparts (tʃ dʒ ʃ ʒ, respectively) before j. The source of this j is either high vowel gliding or the j preceding uw/uː or their unstressed counterpart ə.
Historically, this process occured almost always obligatorily in an unstressed syllable, as in naturenéjtʃə, questionkwéstʃən; procedureprəsíjdʒə, soldiersə́wldʒə; pressurepréʃə, confessionkənféʃən; seizuresíjʒə, fusionfjúwʒən. In a number of these examples, there still are morphologically related other words that exhibit the unpalatalized t d s z: procedeprəsíjd, presspres, seizesijz, etc. In other cases we can only infer from the etymology (and the spelling) of the word that it contained one of these consonants followed by j earlier (compare, for example, nature with its French counterpart natyr or question and French kestjõ).
There are a handful of cases where palatalization occurred historically before a stressed vowel: maturemətʃuː, sureʃuː, etc.
In current English, palatalization is extended in two ways:
it now occurs generally before a stressed vowel too: Tuesdaytjúwzdej > tʃúwzdej, dunedjúwn > dʒúwn (now homophonous with June), assumeəsjúwm > əʃúwm (or əsúwm), presumeprizjúwm > priʒúwm (or prizúwm); note that the two fricatives do not palatalize word initially, so suit can be sjúwt or súwt, but not *ʃúwt, and
palatalization now occurs not only before j but also before other “palatal” (strictly speaking postalveolar) consonants, most commonly r and tʃ: traptʃrap, drawdʒroː, questionkweʃtʃən, disregarddiʃrigaːd, Israeliʒrejl, etc.
In fact, palatalization can occur serially as long as there is a palatalizable consonant, as in stupidstjuwpid > stʃuwpid > ʃtʃuwpid or strongstroŋ > stʃroŋ > ʃtʃroŋ.
the anticlockwise vowel shift
If we look at changes affecting vowels in current British English we can detect a rather general tendency: front vowels lower, low vowels move back, back vowels raise, and high vowels move towards the front.
keyword
Gimson
Lindsey
dress
e
ɛ
trap
æ
a
price
aɪ
ɑj
lot
ɒ
ɔ
force
ɔː
oː
choice
ɔɪ
oj
foot
ʊ
ʉ
The vowel changes shown in the chart are illustrated by the change of vowel transcription symbols from Gimson’s to Lindsey’s (also cf the slides for topic 4). Most vowels change in an anticlockwise direction: front become lower/more open, low vowels become more back, back vowels become higher/more close, and high vowels become more front.The shift is “anticlockwise” because of the arbitrary tradition of having front vowels on the left hand side in the vowel chart. There is one notable exception to this movement of vowels: the vowel part of aw, which although a low vowel moves towards the front (earlier ɑw, current aw).
goose and goat fronting
The fronting of the vowel part of goat (ow > əw) is a British phenomenon, other better-known accents (like General American) retain the earlier ow. Clearly, British English also had ow here as witnessed by the pre-R counterpart of this vowel, oː (eg goatgəwt vs boarboː).In other cases the pre-R vowel is the long version of the vowel part of the diphthong: eg chaintʃejn vs chairtʃeː, cleanklijn vs clearkliː, finefajn vs firefaː. However, we do not observe this phenomenon in CUBE before nonprevocalic l: codekəwd vs coldkowld (vs colakə́wlə), gogəw vs goldgowld (vs Angolaaŋgə́wlə). This is allophonic difference between əw and ow, these are two allophones of the goat vowel.
The fronting of the vowel part of goose (uw > ʉw) is a very similar phenomenon. Note that this change is also indicated by the selection of the CUBE symbol ʉw for goose. Just like in the case of goat-fronting, a nonprevocalic l inhibits goose-fronting too: eg foodfʉwd vs foolfuwl, mutemjʉwt vs mulemjuwl. So the goose vowel also has two allophones, ʉw and uw
L-vocalization
A change affecting the speech of a sizeable part of British speakers is the replacement of nonprevocalic l by w. This change occurs only if the l is not followed by a vowel, thus in millmil > miw, feltfelt > fewt, helphelp > hewp, worldwəːld > wəwd. When before a vowel, l’s remain intact: Milliemilij, fellowfeləw, helloheləw, earlyəːlij.
We can see that the long vowel əː becomes short before a vocalized l, as in world. Diphthongs also lose their offglide in this environment: taletejl > tew, owlawl > aw. That is, ː, j, and w are always lost before L-vocalization.
The outcome of this process is different after stressed and unstressed ə. After the stressed vowel, we find the expected output: nullnə́l > nə́w, however an unstressed ə and a vocalized l yield u: mentalméntəl > méntu, littlelítəl > lítu, Apriléjprəl > éjpru. Interestingly, this changes the phonotactic constraint that restricts the set of word-final vowels to unstressed ə: unstressed u is now also possible word finally.
L-vocalization often results in mergers, the creation of homophonous word pairs. For example, we have seen that tale is now tejw, just like tell.Alternatively, taletéjl > téjəl > téju; feelfíjl > fíjəl > fíju. Likewise, feel may become fíjw, homophonous with fillfil > fiw. The name Calkal > kaw becomes homophonous with cow or gullgə́l > gə́w with go, etc.
Phonemic splits
L-vocalization and goat/goose-fronting together produce two phonemic splits (whereby an earlier single phoneme splits into two new phonemes. This is shown in the following charts.
go
goal
code
cold
earlier
gow
gowl
kowd
kowld
fronting
gəw
↓
kəwd
↓
vocalization
↓
goww
↓
kowwd
now
gəw
gow
kəwd
kowd
We see that the contrast started out as the presence vs absence of an l (present in goal and cold vs absent in go and code). goat-fronting then adds an allophonic difference in the vowels before l by fronting ow to əw, but retaining ow in goal and cold. L-vocalization masks the l, since the w offglide of the diphthong is deleted, thus the w that “comes from” l is indistinguishable from the offglide. The two processes result in the contrast of əw and ow in these (and many other) word pairs, giving rise to a new diphthong phoneme.
too
tool
rude
ruled
earlier
tuw
tuwl
ruwd
ruwld
fronting
tʉw
↓
rʉwd
↓
vocalization
↓
tuww
↓
ruwwd
now
tʉw
tuw
rʉwd
ruwd
A very similar split occurs in the case of uw: fronting adds allophonic ʉw to the system, and L-vocalization masks the cause of the allophony, l, leaving behind contrasting, ie phonemic, uw vs ʉw. The process is detailed in the chart to the right.
The əw–ow and ʉw–uw splits may occur even without L vocalization. There are speakers who have the first of these pairs before a prevocalic l, but the second if the vowel is in the next morpheme: so holy is həwlij (since this is a single morpheme), but holey is howlij (since here the stem holehowl is followed bythe suffix ij, the l is not directly followed by the vowel). Similarly, the words ruler (for drawing lines) is rʉwlə, while ruler (one who rules) is ruwlə. This increases the number of contrasting diphthongs in English.
goose and goat glide fronting
A very recent, therefore not so widespread novelty is the fronting of the glide portion of goose and goat. This means that goosegʉws becomes gʉjs and goatgəwt becomes gəjt.